Los autores que publiquen en esta Revista aceptan las siguientes condiciones:
Acta Colombiana de Psicología se guía por las normas internacionales sobre propiedad intelectual y derechos de autor, y de manera particular el artículo 58 de la Constitución Política de Colombia, la Ley 23 de 1982 y el Acuerdo 172 del 30 de Septiembre de 2010 (Reglamento de propiedad intelectual de la Universidad Católica de Colombia)
Los autores conservan los derechos de autor y ceden a la Revista el derecho de la primera publicación, con el trabajo registrado con la Este obra está bajo una licencia de Creative Commons Reconocimiento-NoComercial-CompartirIgual 4.0 Internacional, que permite a terceros utilizar lo publicado siempre que mencionen la autoría del trabajo y a la primera publicación en esta Revista.
Resumen
Aunque muchas de las investigaciones del análisis experimental del comportamiento (AEC) están basadas en animales de laboratorio, actualmente está aumentando la investigación referida al comportamiento humano. El comportamiento de las personas ocurre en un entorno social. Una de las principales tareas del AEC es mostrar como la conducta individual es adquirida, mantenida, y modificada mediante la interacción con otros. Otra tarea sería investigar cómo afectan los grupos sociales y la sociedad a la conducta del individuo. Recientemente, los analistas de la conducta han centrado su atención en factores relacionados con problemas de conducta en humanos (Pierce y Epling, 1995). En el momento actual, los problemas de la conducta alimentaria, también llamados trastornos de la alimentación, se consideran síndromes y por tanto, suelen definirse en función de la aparición de un grupo de síntomas. Los trastornos de la alimentación en general, y en concreto la anorexia nerviosa, han aumentado en su incidencia de una forma vertiginosa en las sociedades occidentales. Anorexia significa falta de apetito y es un término que se utiliza habitualmente para designar un síntoma que aparece en muchos trastornos y enfermedades, como por ejemplo la depresión o la gripe.
Citas
Agras, W. S. (1987). Eating disorders: Management of obbesity, bulimia, and anorexia nervosa. New York: Pergamon Press.
American Psyquiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Stadistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd. ed. rev.). Wahington, DC:Author.
Aravich, P.F. (1996). Advers effects of exercise stress and restricted feeding in the rat: Theorical and neurobiological considerations. En En W. F. Epling y W. D. Pierce (Eds), Activity Anorexia: Theory, Research and Treatment, (pp. 81-97). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: PrenticeHall.
Beck, S.B., Ward-Hull, C.I. y McLerar, P.M. (1976). Variables related to woman´s somatic preferences of the male and female body. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 1200-1210.
Beneke, W. M., Schulte, S. E. y Vander Tuig, J. G. (1995). An analysis of excessive running in the development of activity anorexia. Physiology and Behavior, 58, 451-457.
Beumont, A.L. (1991). Forward to solving the anorexia puzzle: A scientiffic approach. En W.F. Epling y W.D. Pierce (Eds.), Soving the anorexia puzzle: A scientific Aproach. 9-15. Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber.
Boer, D. P. (1989). Determinants of excessive activity in anorexia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uniersity of Alberta, Edmonton.
Boer, D. P., Epling, W. F., Pierce, W. D. y Russell, J. C. (1990). Suppresion of food deprivation induced high-rate wheel running in rats. Physiology and Behavior, 48, 339-342.
Boakes, R. A. y Dywer, D.M. (1997). Weight loss in rats produced by running: Effects of prior experience and individual housing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 50B, 129-148.
Boakes, R. y Juraskova I. (2001). The role of drinking in the suppression of food intake by recent activity. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 718-731.
Boakes, R. A., Mills, K.J., y Single, J.P. (1999). Sex Differences in the relationship between activity and weight loss in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113, 1-10.
Bolles, R. C. y De Lorge, J. (1962). The rats adjustment to a-diurnal feeding cycles. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 760-762.
Bolles, R. C. y Stokes, L. W. (1965). Rat´s anticipation of diurnal and a-diurnal feeding. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 60, 290-294.
Brigham, J.C. (1980). Limiting conditions of the “physical attractiveness stereotype”: Attributions about divorces. Journal of Research in Personality, 14, 365-375.
Bruch, H. (1973). Eating disoder: Obesity anorexia nervosa and the person within. Nueva York. Basic Books.
Chaoulof, F. (1989). Physical exercise and brain monoamines: a review. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 137, 1-13.
Dishman, R. K. (1997). Brain monoamines, exercise, and behavioral stress: animal models. Medicine and Sicence in Sport and Ejercise, 3, 27-74.
Dwyer, D. M. y Boakes, R. A. (1997). Activitybased anorexia in rats as failure to adapt to feeding schedule. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 195-205.
Edholm, O. G., Fletcher, J. G., Widdowson, E.M. y McCance, R. A. (1955). The energy expenditure and food intake of individual men. Britisth Journal of Nutrition, 9, 286-300.
Epling, W. F. y Pierce, W. D. (1984). Activity based anorexia inrats as a function of opportunity to run on an activity wheel. Nutrition and Behavior, 2, 37-49.
Epling, W. F. y Pierce, W. D. (1988). Activitybased anorexia: A biobehavioral perspective. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 475-485.
Epling, W. F. y Pierce, W. D. (1992). Solving the anorexia puzzle: A scientific approach. Toronto: Hogrefe & Huhuber.
Falk, J. L. (1961). Production of polidipsia in normal rats by an intermittent food schedule. Science, 133, 195-196.
Falk, J. L. (1967).Control of schedule-inducedpolydipsia: Type, size and spacing of meals, Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 199-206.
Flores, P. y Pellón, R. (1995) Effects of Drugs on the temporal distribution of schedule induced polydipsia in rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 43, 689-695.
Flores, P., Lamas, E. y Pellón, R. (1995). Polidipsia inducida por el programa y conductas adjuntivas, Ciencia Psicológica, 1, 24-45.
Frish, R. E., Wyshank, G., y Vicent, L. (1980). Delayed menarche and amenorrhea ind balett dancers. The New England of Medicine, 303, 17-19.
Garner, D.M., Rockert, W., Olmstead, M.P., Johnson, C. y Coscina, D.V. (1985). Psychoeducational principles in the treatment of bulimia and anorexia nervosa. En D.M. Garner y P. E. Gargingel (Eds). Handbook of Psychotherapy forAnorexia Nervosa and Bulimia (pp. 513-572). New York: Guilford.
Green, S.K., Buchanan, D.R. y Heuer, S.K. (1984). Winners, losers, and chosers: A field investigation of dating initiation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 502-511.
Halmi, K. A. (1974). Anorexia nervosa: Demographic and clinical features. Psychosomatic Medicine, 36, 18-26.
Halmi, K. A. (1985). Behavioral management for anorexia nervosa. En D. M. Garner y P. E. Garfinkle (Eds.). Handbook of psychoteraphy for anorexia and bulimia (147-159). New York: The Gilford Press.
Jones, G. H., Robins, T. W., y Marsden, Ch., A. (1989). Isolation rearing retards the acquisition of schedule induced polydipsia in rats. Physiology and Behavior, 45, 71-77.
Kanarek R. B. y Collier, G. (1983). Self-Starvation: A problem of overriding the satiety signal? Physiology and Behavior, 30, 307-311.
Katz, J.L. (1986). Long distance running, anorexia nervose, and bulimia: A report of two cases. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 27, 74-78.
Kaye, W. H., Picker, D. M., Naber, D. y Ebert, M. H. (1982). Cerebroespinal fluid opioid activity in anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, (643-645).
Lakoff, R.T. y Scherr, R.L. (1984). Face Value: The politics of beauty. Boston: Rouletge.
Lambert, K. G. (1993). The activity stress paradigm: Possible mechanisms and applications. The Journal of General Psychology, 120, 21-32.
Lett, B.T. y Grant, V.L. (1996). Wheel running induces conditioned taste aversion in rats trained while hungry and thirsty. Physiology and Behavior, 59, 699-702.
Lett, B.T., Grant, V.L. y Gaborko, L.L. (1998). Wheel running simultaneously induces CTA and facilitates feeding in non-deprived rats. Appetite, 31, 351-360.
Lett, B.T., Grant, V.L., Smith, J.F. y Koh, M.T. (2001). Preadaptation to the feeding schedule does not eliminate activity -based anorexia in rats. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54B, 193-199.
Levine, R. y Levine, S. (1989). Role of the pituitary adrenal hormones in the acquisition of schedule induced polydipsia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 621-637.
Miller, T.M., Coffman, J.G. y Linke, R.A. (1980). Survey on body image, weight and diet of college students. Journal of American Dietetic Association, 77, 561-566.
Morrow, N. S. y Garrick, T. (1993). Effects of preadaptation to restricted feeding and metidine treatment on gastric mucosal injury and wheel-running during exposure to activitystress ulcer in the rat. Journal of Psychology, 87, 245-252.
Palya, W. L. (1993). Bipolar control in fixed interfood intervals. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 345-359.
Paré, W. P. (1975).The influence of food consumption and running activity on the activity stress ulcer in the rat. American Journal of Digestive Disease, 20, 262-273.
Pellón, R. (1990). Polidipsia inducida por el programa: I. Definición y marco conceptual. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 43, 313-326.
Pellón, R. (1992). Polidipsia inducida por el programa: II. Variables motivacionales. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 45, 251-265.
Pellón, R. Y Blackman, D. E. (1992) Effects of drugs on the temporal distribution of Schedule-Induced Polydipsia in Rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 43, 689-695.
Pellón, R., Flores, P. y Blackman, D. E. (1998). Influencias ambientales sobre la conducta inducida por programa. En R. Ardila, W. López, A. M. Pérez, y F. Reyes (Eds.). Manual de Análysis Experimental del Comportamiento. Madrid: Biblioteca Nueva.
Perpiñá, C. (1995). Trastornos Alimentarioa. En Belloch, A., Sandín, B. Ramos, F. Manual de Psicopatología. McGrau-Hill, 1, 531-555.
Pierce, W. D. y Epling, W. F. (1991). Activity anorexia: An animal model and theory of human self-starvation. En A. Boulton, G. Baker y M. Martin-Iverson (Eds.). Neuromethods: Animal models in psychiatry, 63, 2549-2553.
Pierce, W.D. y Epling, W.F. (1994). An interplay between basic and applied behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 17, 7-23.
Pierce, W. D. y Epling, W. F. (1995). Theorical Developments in Activity Anorexia. En W. F. Epling y W. D., Pierce. (Eds). Activity Anorexia: Theory, Research, and Treament, 3, 23-41.
Pierce, W. D. y Epling, W. F. (1995). Behavior Analysis and Learning. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Yersey.
Pierce, W.D., Epling, W.F. y Boer, D.P. (1986). Deprivation and satiation: The interrelations between food and wheel running. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 46, 199-210.
Rieg, T.S., Maestrello, A.M. y Aravich, P.F. (1994). Weight cycling alters the effects of d-fenfluramine on susceptibility to activitybased anorexia. American Jounal of Clinical Nutrition, 60, 494-500.
Routtenberg, A. y Kuznesof, A. Y. (1967). Selfstarvation of rats living in activity wheels on a restricted feeding Schedule. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 64, 414-421.
Russell-Davis, D. (1951). Studies in malnutrition. M.R.C. special report seres no. 275. London: H.M. Stationery Office.
Tanford, S. y Penrod, S. (1984). Social influence model: A formal integration of research on mayority and minority influence processes. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 189-225.
Tang, M., Willians, S. L. y Falk, J. L. (1988). Prior schedule exposure reduces the acquisition of schedule-induced polydipsia. Physiology and Behavior, 44, 817-820.
Treasure, J. (1991). Long-term management of eating disoder. International Review of Psychiatry, 3, 43-58.
Watanabe, K., Hara, C., y Ogawa, N. (1992). Feeding conditions and estrous cycle of female rats under the activity-stress procedure from aspect of anorexia nervosa. Psychology and Behavior, 51, 827-833.