Authors who publish in this journal agree to the following terms:
Acta Colombiana de Psicología complies with international intellectual property and copyright laws, and particularly with Article No. 58 of the Political Constitution of Colombia, Law No. 23 of 1982, and the Agreement No. 172 of September 30, 2010 (Universidad Católica de Colombia Intellectual Property Regulation).
Authors retain their copyright and grant to the Acta Colombiana de Psicología the right of first publication, with the work registered under Creative Commons attribution license, which allows third parties to use the published material, provided they credit the authorship of the work and the first publication in this Journal.
Abstract
Thirty college students were exposed to accurate instructions and continuous (Experiments 1 and 2) or delayed (Experiment 3) feedback during training in conditional discrimination tasks. The effectiveness of instructional control on transfer-test performance was evaluated. Subjects received one of two training sequences (similarity/difference or difference/ similarity) with first-order matching to sample. Transfer tests (similarity or difference) were presented after each training phase (similarity or difference); a final transfer test (similarity and difference) was presented after the entire training sequence was completed. Results showed effective performances during training and initial transfer test; but performance in a final test with major requirements for transfer was poorer (Experiments 1 and 2). In Experiment 3 the transfer -test instructions highlighting previous training were introduced, and effective performance resulted. These data suggest that the characteristics of stimuli, instructions and feedback are relevant variables interacting for the instructional control and the transfer of effective performances in conditional discrimination tasks.
Keywords:
References
Ayllon, T., & Azrin N. H. (1964). Reinforcement and instructions with mental patients. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 327-331.
Baron, A., & Galizio, M. (1983). Instructional control of human operant behavior. Psychological Record, 33, 495-520.
Baron, A., Kaufman, A., & Stauber, K. A. (1969). Effects of instructions and reinforcement-feedback on human operant behavior maintained by fixed-interval reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 701-712.
Buskist, W., F., Bennett, R. H., & Miller, H. L., Jr. (1981). Effects of instructional constrains on human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 217-225.
Buskist, W. F., & Miller, H. L., Jr. (1986). Interaction between rules and contingencies in the control of human fixed-interval performance. The Psychological Record, 36, 109-116.
Danforth J. S., Chase, P. N., Dolan, M., & Joyce, J. H. (1990). The establishment of stimulus control by instructions and by differential reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 97-112.
Degrandpre, R. J. y Buskist, W. F. (1991). Effects of accuracy of instructions on human behavior: Correspondance with reinforcement contingencies matters. The Psychological Record, 41, 371-384.
Dermer, M. L., y Rodgers, J. G. (1997). Schedule control over following instructions comprised of novel combinations of verbal stimuli. The Psychological Record, 47, 243-260.
Dixon, M. R., y Hayes, L. J. (1998). Effects of differing instructional histories on the resurgence of rule-following. The Psychological Record, 48, 272-292.
Goldiamond, I. (1966). Perception, language, and conceptualization. En: Kleinmuntz, B. (Ed.), Problem solving: Research, Method, and Theory (pp. 183-224). New York: Wiley.
LeFrancois, J. R., Chase, P. N., & Joyce, J. H. (1988). The effects of a variety of instructions on human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 383-393.
Martínez, H. (1996). El concepto de covariación: un modelo y una propuesta. Revista Iberoamericana de Psicología, 4, 52-60.
Martínez, H. (2001). Estudios sobre transferencia en comportamiento humano. En: G. Mares y Y. Guevara (Eds.) Psicología interconductual vol 1. México, UNAM.
Martínez, H., González, A., Ortiz, A., y Carrillo, K. (1998). Aplicación de un modelo de covariación al análisis de las ejecuciones de sujetos humanos en condiciones de entrenamiento y de transferencia en una tarea de discriminación condicional. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 30, 233-260.
Martínez, H., y Moreno, R. (1995). El papel de la contigüidad y la contingencia en el condicionamiento: una revisión y una propuesta metodológica. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 21, 75-106.
Martínez, H., Moreno, R., Ortiz, G. y Carrillo, K. (1996, Agosto). Eficiencia en la ejecución: una aplicación del modelo de covariación. Trabajo presentado en el XXVI Congress International de Psychologie, Montreal, Canadá.
Martínez, H. y Ribes, E. (1996). Interactions of contingencies and instructional history on conditional discrimination. The Psychological Record, 46, 301-318.
Newman, B., Buffington, D. M., y Hemmes, N. S. (1995). The efffects of schedules of reinforcement on instruction following. The Psychological Record, 45, 463-476.
Ribes, E. y Martínez, H. (1990). Interaction of contingencies and rule instructions in the performance of human participants in conditional discrimination. The Psychological Record, 40, 565-586.
Ribes, E., y López, F. (1985). Teoría de la conducta: un análisis de campo y paramétrico. Trillas: México.
Schlinger, H., y Blakely, E. (1987). Functionaltering effects of contingency-specifying stimuli. Behavior Analyst, 10, 41-45.
Schwartz, B. (1982). Reinforcement-induced response stereotypy: How not to teach people to discover rules. Journal of the Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 23-59.
Zentall, T. R. (1997). Animal memory: The role of “intructions”. Learning and Motivation, 28, 280-308.