How to Cite
Cadavid Ruiz, N., & del Río, P. (2012). Verbal working memory and its relationship with socio-demographic variables in colombian childrens. Acta Colombiana De Psicología, 15(1), 99–109. Retrieved from https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/200
License

 Authors who publish in this journal agree to the following terms:

 Acta Colombiana de Psicología complies with international intellectual property and copyright laws, and particularly with Article No. 58 of the Political Constitution of Colombia, Law No. 23 of 1982, and the Agreement No. 172 of September 30, 2010 (Universidad Católica de Colombia Intellectual Property Regulation).

 Authors retain their copyright and grant to the Acta Colombiana de Psicología the right of first publication, with the work registered under Creative Commons attribution license, which allows third parties to use the published material, provided they credit the authorship of the work and the first publication in this Journal.

Abstract

The increasing inclusion of socio-demographic variables in studies of cognitive development in children points to a renewed interest in the scientific community to identify how context shapes human development. Along this line of knowledge, this study aims to examine the relationship between development of verbal working memory, age, socioeconomic status, type of school and daily activities performed by 159 Colombian children aged 6 and 8 years old. Their verbal working memory was assessed using the Progression and Regression Digit Span Test, and the Letters and Numbers Tests of the WISC-IV. Results show that older children perform better in both tests, and that children of middle and upper classes who attend private schools are those who score higher on the Progression and Regression Digit Span Test. In addition, it was found that activities like using the computer, practicing sports and doing homework allow a better performance in the above mentioned task. Results of this study suggest that the Progression and Regression Digit Span task depends on socio-demographic variables, while the Letters and Numbers Test does not seem to be linked to them.

Keywords:

References

Alzate, M. C. (2006). La estratificación socioeconómica para el cobro de los servicios públicos domiciliarios en Colombia ¿Solidaridad o focalización? Santiago de Chile: Publicación de las Naciones Unidas. Recuperado de http://www.dane.gov.co/files/geoestadistica/Estratificacion_Solidaridad_CEPAL.pdf.

Akhutina, T. V. (1997). The remediation of executive functions in children with cognitive disorders: the Vygotski-Luria neuropsychological approach. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 41(2), 144-151.

Akhutina, T. V. (2003). L.S. Vygotski and A.R. Luria: Foundations of Neuropsychology. Journal of Russian y East European Psychology, 41(3), 159-190.

Anderson, V. A. (2001). Assessing executive functions in children: biological, psychological, and developmental considerations. Pediatric rehabilitation, 4(3), 119-136.

Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., y Guajardo, S. (2005). The Influence of the Parents? Educational Level on the Development of Executive Functions. Developmental neuropsychology, 28(1), 539-560.

Baddeley, A. D. (1987). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working Memory. Science, 31, 556- 559.

Baddeley, A. D. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485-493.

Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., y Papagano, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105(1), 158-173.

Barkley, R. A. (1998). A theory of ADHD: Inhibition, executive functions, self-control, and time. In G. Reid Lyon y N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 225-260). New York: Guilford Press.

Brocki, K. C. y Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental neuropsychology, 26(2), 571-593.

Brown, A. B., Biederman, J., Valera, E., Makris, N., Doyle, A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., et al. (2011). Relationship of DAT1 and adult ADHD to task-positive and task-negative working memory networks. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 193(1), 7-16.

Bull, R. y Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, task switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 273-293.

Cain, K., Oakhill, J.y Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31-42.

Carey, S., Gelman, R.y Jean Piaget Society (1991). The Epigenesis of mind: essays on biology and cognition. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F. y González de Sather, J. C. M. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: Examination of task-switching performance. Developmental psychology, 37(5), 715-730.

Departamento Administrativo Nacional de Estadística (s.f.) Preguntas frecuentes Estratificación Socioeconómica. Recuperado de http://www.dane.gov.co/files/geoestadistica/Preguntas_frecuentes_estratificación.pdf

Del Río, P.y Alvarez, A. (1992). Content analysis of the television watched by spanish children. Unpublished report. Televisión Española.

Gathercole, S. E., Alloway, T. P., Willis, C., y Adamas, A. M. (2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(3), 265-281.

Gelman, R.y Williams, E. M. (1998). Enabling constraints for cognitive development and learning: Domain specificity and epigenesis. In William Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 575-630). Hoboken: John Wiley y Sons Inc.

Gottlieb, G. (2002). From gene to organism: The developing individual as an emergent, interactional, hierarchical system. In Mark H. Johnson, Yuko Munakata y Rick O. Gilmore (Eds.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (2 ed., pp. 36-49). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 233-253.

Josiane-Engel, P. M., Santos, F. H., y Gathercole, S. E. (2008). Are Working Memory Measures Free of Socioeconomic Influence. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 51, 1580-1587.

Korkman, M. (2001). Introduction to the Special Issue on Normal Neuropsychological Development in the School-Age Years. Developmental neuropsychology, 20(1), 325-330.

Korkman, M., Kemp, S. L. y Kirk, U. (2001). Effects of age on neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: A crosssectional study on 800 children from the United States. Developmental neuropsychology, 20(1), 331-354.

Law, N., Bouffet, E., Laughlin, S., Laperriere, N., Briere, M. E., Strother, D. et al., (2011). Cerebello-thalamo-cerebral connections in pediatric brain tumor patients: Impact on working memory. NeuroImage, 56(4), 2238-2248.

Lyon, G. R. (1996). The Need for Conceptual and Theoretical Clarity in the Study of Attention, Memory, and Executive Function. In G. Reid Lyon y Norman A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function. (pp. 3-9). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist, 53(2), 185-204.

Nash, H.y Heath, J. (2011). The role of vocabulary, working memory and inference making ability in reading comprehension in down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1782-1791.

Ostrosky-Solís, F., y Lozano, A. (2006). Digit Span: Effect of Education and Culture. International Journal of Psychology, 41(5), 333-341.

Patton, N., McDonald, C., Thomas-Tate, S.y Love, M (2010). Examining relationships among dialect variation, literacy skills and school context in first grade. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 53, 126-145.

Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O.y Roberts, R. J. Jr. (1996). Executive functions and working memory: Theoretical and measurement issues. In G. Reid Lyon y Norman A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 327-348). Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

Phillips, B. M., y Lonigan, C. J. (2009). Variations in the Home Literacy: Environment of Preschool Children: A cluster analytic approach. scientific Studies of Reading, 13(2), 146-174.

Reynolds, C. R., Willson, V. L., y Ramsey, M. (1999). Intellectual differences among mexican americans, papagos and whites, indepent of g. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(1181- 1187), 1181.

Schuchardt, K., Maehler, C.y Hasselhorn, M. (2011). Functional deficits in phonological working memory in children with intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1934-1940.

Swanson, H. L. (2003). Age-related differences in learning disabled and skilled reader?s working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(1), 1-31.

Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D?Onofrio, B.y Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6), 623-628.

Vygotski, L. S. (1987). Pensamiento y lenguaje: Teoría del desarrollo cultural de las funciones psíquicas. Buenos Aires: La Pleyade.

Wechsler, D. (2005). WISC-IV Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para niños-IV. Adaptación española. Madrid: TEA.

Wechsler, D. (2007). WISC-IV Escala de inteligencia para niños IV ? Versión estandarizada. México, D.F.: Manual Moderno.

Reference by

Sistema OJS 3 - Metabiblioteca |