How to Cite
Pérez Manrique, T., Diaz Barbosa, J. H., & Pulido Vega, J. C. (2009). Behavioral contrast in relational operants with human participants. Acta Colombiana De Psicología, 12(2), 115–126. Retrieved from https://actacolombianapsicologia.ucatolica.edu.co/article/view/283
License

 Authors who publish in this journal agree to the following terms:

 Acta Colombiana de Psicología complies with international intellectual property and copyright laws, and particularly with Article No. 58 of the Political Constitution of Colombia, Law No. 23 of 1982, and the Agreement No. 172 of September 30, 2010 (Universidad Católica de Colombia Intellectual Property Regulation).

 Authors retain their copyright and grant to the Acta Colombiana de Psicología the right of first publication, with the work registered under Creative Commons attribution license, which allows third parties to use the published material, provided they credit the authorship of the work and the first publication in this Journal.

Abstract

Behavioral contrast is a phenomenon which characterizes multiple programs with common operants such as lever pressures or key pecks. It must be pointed out that behavioral contrast is widely documented in animals. Recently, different procedures have been developed which might be applied to the study of behavioral contrast with relational operants (i.e., conditional discrimination). The purpose of this research was to determine whether behavioral contrast takes place when working with this type of operants. Six high school students, who volunteered to participate and did not receive any other payment than the one arranged by the contingencies of the reinforcement schedule were exposed to a multiple program of 100 trials per component. After this procedure, a test consisting of derivative relations without reinforcement associated to each choice was carried out. When analyzing the reason for choice by component in Experiment 1, a positive contrast in the three participants was found and in the relational test, transference of preference was evidenced in participants 1 and 2. In Experiment 2, negative induction was found in the three participants and in the transference test, a lower degree of negative induction was evidenced. The conclusion drawn from this experiment is that change in the contingencies can be placed under contextual control in the manner of a relational framework.

Keywords:

References

Amsel, A. (1962).Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning: Some recent history and a theoretical extension. Psychological Review, 69: 306-328.

Antonitis, J. (1951). Response variability in the white rat during conditioning, extinction, and reconditioning. Journal of Experimental. Psychology. 42: 273–281.

Bloomfield, T. M. (1969). Behavioral contrast and the peak shift. En R. M. Gilbert y N. S. Sutherland (Eds.). Animal discrimination learning (pp. 215-241). New York: Academic Press.

Bolles, R. C. (1991). The hedonics of taste .Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.

Catania, A. C. (1969). Concurrent performances: inhibition of one response by reinforcement of another. Journal of the Experimental analysis of Behavior. 12: 731-744.

Dymond, S. & Barnes, D. (1995). A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, more than, and less than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64:163–184.

Edwards, R. P. (1979). Behavioral contrast in humans with response-independent reinforcement. Journal of General Psychology, 100: 159-160.

Epstein, R. (1985). Extinction-induced resurgence: Preliminary investigations and possible applications. Psychological Record, 35: 143–153.

Epstein, R. (1986). Simulation research in the analysis of behavior. En A. Poling y R. W.

Epstein, R. & Skinner, B. F. (1980). Resurgence of responding after the cessation of response- independent reinforcement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 77: 6251–6253.

Epstein, R. & Skinner, B. F. (1981). The spontaneous use of memoranda by pigeons. Behaviour Analysis Letters, 1: 241-246.

Ferster, C. B. & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall. Inc. Fuqua (Eds.), Research methods in applied behavior analysis (pp. 127-155). New York: Plenum Press.

Gamzu, E. & Williams, D. R. (1971). Classical conditioning of a complex skeletal act. Science, 171: 923-925.

Hanson, H. M. (1959). Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 58: 321-333.

Hantula, D. A. & Crowell, C. R. (1994). Behavioral contrast in a two option analogue task of financial decision making. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27: 607-617.

Hayes, C. S. (1994). Relational Frame Theory: A functional approach to verbal events. En S. C. Hayes, L. J. Hayes, M. Sato & K. One (Eds.), Behavior analysis of language and cognition (pp. 9-30). Reno, NV: Context Press.

Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 13: 243-266.

Herrnstein, R. J. & Hineline, P. N. (1966). Negative reinforcement as shock – frequency reduction. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 9: 421-430.

Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Keehn, J. D. (1970). Reinforcement of alcoholism: schedule control of solitary drinking. Quarter y Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 31: 28-39.

MacCorquodale, K. & Meehl, P. E. (1948). On a distinction between hypothetical constructs and intervening variables. Psychological Review, 55: 95-107.

McIlvane, W. J., Kledaras, J. B., Munson, L. C., King, K. A., de Rose, J. C. & Stoddard, L. T. (1987). Controlling relations in conditional discrimination and matching by exclusion. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48: 187-208.

Mowrer, O. H. (1940). An experimental analogue of ‘‘regression’’ with incidental observations on ‘‘reaction formation.’’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 35: 56-87.

O’Kelly, L. I. (1940). An experimental study of regression. I. Behavioral characteristics of the regressive response. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 30: 41-53.

Pettenger, D. J., Pavlik, W. B., Flora, S. R. & Kontos, J. M. (1988). The persistence of learned behaviors in humans as a function of changes in reinforcement schedule and response. Learning and Motivation, 19: 300-316.

Premack, D. (1965). Reinforcement theory. En D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 123-180). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Rawson, R. A., Leitenberg, H., Mulick, J. A & Lefebvre, M .F. (1977). Recovery of extinction responding in rats following discontinuation of reinforcement of alternative behavior: A test of two explanations. Animal Learning and Behavior, 5: 415–420.

Reynolds, G. S. (1961). Behavioral contrast. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4: 57-71.

Sajwah, T., Twardosz, S. & Burke, M. (1972). Side effects of extinction procedures in a remedial school. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5: 163–175.

Sanders, M. J. (1937). An experimental demonstration of regression in the rat. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21: 493-510.

Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of speech and Hearing Research, 14: 5-13.

Sidman, M., Cresson, O. & Willson-Morris, M. (1974). Acquisition of matching-to-sample via mediated transfer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22: 261-273.

Simmons, R. (1924). The relative effectiveness of certain incentives in animal learning. Comparative Psychology Monographs. 11, n 7: 1-79.

Skinner, B. F. (1938/1975). La conducta de los organismos: un análisis experimental. Barcelona: Fontanella.

Skinner, B. F. (1957/1981). Conducta verbal. México: Trillas.

Skinner, B. F. (1984). Selection by consequences. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7: 477-510.

Spencer, H. (1870). Principles of Psychology. 2nd Ed. London: (En Boakes, 1984)

Spradlin, J. E., Cotter, V. W. & Baxley, N. (1973). Establishing a conditional discrimination without direct training: A study of transfer with retarded adolescents. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 77: 556–566.

Staddon, J. R. & Simmelhag, V. L. (1971). The superstition experiment: a reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review, 78: 3-43.

Steele, D. L. & Hayes, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56: 519– 555.

Stromer, R., Mcilvane, W. J. & Serna, R. W. (1993). Complex stimulus control and equivalence. The Psychological Record. 43: 585-598.

Tarbox, J. & Hayes, L. P. (2005). Verbal behavior and behavioral contrast in human subjects. The Psychological Record, 55: 419-437.

Terrace, H. S. (1974). On the nature of non-responding in discrimination learning with and without errors. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 22: 151-159.

Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York: Macmillan.

Tinklepaugh, O. L. (1928). An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. Journal of the comparative Psychology, 8: 197-236.

Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Century.

Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological review, 20: 158-177.

Williams, B, A. (2002). Behavioral contrast redux. Animal Learning & Behavior, 30: 1-20.

Reference by

Sistema OJS 3 - Metabiblioteca |