Authors who publish in this journal agree to the following terms:
Acta Colombiana de Psicología complies with international intellectual property and copyright laws, and particularly with Article No. 58 of the Political Constitution of Colombia, Law No. 23 of 1982, and the Agreement No. 172 of September 30, 2010 (Universidad Católica de Colombia Intellectual Property Regulation).
Authors retain their copyright and grant to the Acta Colombiana de Psicología the right of first publication, with the work registered under Creative Commons attribution license, which allows third parties to use the published material, provided they credit the authorship of the work and the first publication in this Journal.
Abstract
This systematic review of the literature on the study of writing disorders in Parkinson disease (PD) evaluated the available evidence with respect to research about writing and related cognitive processes in this population. A critical analysis of the literature was carried out, where the databases from Pubmed, Medline, Hinari, PsycArticles, E Journals and Science Direct, were accessed using as search criteria the publication date range between January 1, 1999 until October 1, 2009, with the exception of an article of 1997 which was included because of the contribution it provides on linguistic analysis. Search indicators were: clinical trials, reviews, meta-analysis, controlled clinical trials and discussions. A total of 39 articles were recorded, among which 15 were chosen based on their relevance and relationship with the topic of writing in PD. Six of these had taken into account linguistic variables in exclusively oral tasks; only one of them clearly describes the correlation of the grammatical structure of sentences within the writing linguistic tasks. Findings show that within the oral/linguistic studies of subjects with PD there is a predominance of correlations between neurobiological factors and alterations of oral language in comprehension tasks of semantics, syntax, grammatical and ungrammatical sentences, use of words in discourse and interaction in context. Studies of writing focus on mechanical aspects, including changes in size and modification of strokes in the handwriting of PD patients. Only one study is directly related to a linguistic analysis of writing, where parameters such as sentence length, syntactic complexity and lexical-semantic content were considered. The writing process in PD patients has been analyzed from the mechanical disturbance, that is, from the physical form of strokes and has been correlated with impaired motor functions underlying the main motor dysfunction in PD, and although an associated cognitive impairment has been acknowledged, this correlation has not extended to the writing process.
Keywords:
References
Angwin A., Chenery H., Copland D., Murdoch B. y Silburn P. (2007). The speed of lexical activation is altered in Parkinson’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol; 29(1): 73-85.
Benito-León J., Bermejo, F.,Molina J. (1998). Criterios diagnósticos de la enfermedad de Párkinson y su influencia sobre la prevalencia de la enfermedad en estudios poblacionales. Neurología. Enero 13 (1) 33:39.
Boulenger V., Mechtouff L., Thobois S., Broussolle E., Jeannerod M. y Nazir T. (2008). Word processing in Parkinson’s disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns. Neuropsychologia. Jan 31;46(2):743-56.
Breitenstein, C., Daum, I., y Ackermann, H. (1998). Emotionalprocessing following cortical and subcortical brain damage: Contribution of the fronto-striatal circuitry. Behavioural Neurology, 11, 29–42.
Breitenstein, C., Van Lancker, D., Daum, I., y Waters, C. (2001). Impaired perception of vocal emotions in Parkinson’s disease: Influence of speech time processing andexecutive functioning. Brain and Cognition, 45, 277–314.
Caixeta L., SoaresV.L., Soares C.D. y Reis G. (2009). Dementia and mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatria. Sep;67(3B):959.
Cassinelli D., Santibáñez R. (2004) Relación entre manifestaciones motoras y deterioro cognitivo en la enfermedad de Párkinson. Revista Ecuatoriana de Neurología. Volumen 13 (3).
Chaudhuri K., Healy, D. y Schapira A. (2006) Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease: diagnosis and Management. Lancet Neurol 2006; 5: 235–45.
Factor S. y Weiner W.J. (2008). Parkinson’s disease: diagnosis and clinical management. Sheridan Press Ed. New York, NY, EE.UU.
Fahn A. (2003). Description of Parkinson’s Disease as a Clinical Syndrome. New York Academy of Sciences 991: 1–14.
Fernández W. (2005). Trastornos Extrapiramidales. Medicina Interna Chalem (Cáp. 184). (3ra. ed.). Bogotá, Ed. Guadalupe
Frades B., Guerrero-Díaz, M. y Martínez P. (2004) Alteraciones neuropsicológicas en la enfermedad de Parkinson: evaluación de impacto sobre el cuidador. Revista de neurología, Vol. 39, Nº. 7, 2004, págs. 639-645.
Friederici A., Kotz S., Werheid K., Hein G. y Cramon D. (2003). Syntactic Comprehension in Parkinson’s Disease: Investigating Early Automatic and Late Integrational Processes Using Event-Related Brain Potentials. Neuropsychology. 2003, Vol. 17, No. 1, 133–142.
Gangadhar G., Joseph D., Srinivasan A., Subramanian D., Shivakeshavan R.G., Shobana N. y Chakravarthy V. (2009). A computational model of Parkinsonian handwriting that highlights the role of the indirect pathway in the basal ganglia. Hum Mov Sci. Oct;28(5):602-18.
Gelb D.J., Oliver E., Gilman S. (1999). Diagnostic criteria for Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 1999 Jan;56(1):33-9.
Gotham A. M., Brown, R. G., y Marsden, C. D. (1988). “Frontal” cognitive function in patients with Parkinson’s disease “on” and “off” levodopa. Brain, 111, 299–321.
Grossman M., Carvell S., Gollomp S., Stern M. B., Vernon R. N. y Hurtig, H. I. (1991). Sentence comprehension andpraxis deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 41, 1620–1626.
Grossman M., Carvell S., Stern M. B., Gollomp S. y Hurtig, H. I. (1992). Sentence comprehension in Parkinson’s disease: The role of attention and memory. Brain and Language, 42,347–384.
Grossman M., Cooke D., DeVita C., Lee C., Alsop D., Detre J., Gee J., Chen W., Stern B. y Hurtig H. (2003). Grammatical and resource components of sentence processing in Parkinson’s disease: An fMRI study. Neurology. 2003 Mar 11;60(5):775-8.
Holtgraves T., McNamara P., Cappaert K. yDurso R. (2009). Linguistic correlates of asymmetric motor symptom severity in Parkinson’s Disease. Brain Cogn. Mar;72(2):189-96.
Jarzebska E. (2006). Evaluation of effectiveness of the micrographia’s therapy in Parkinson’s disease patients. Pol Merkur Lekarski. Jun;20(120):688-90.
Kim E., Lee B., Park C., Lee W. y. Na D. (2005). Micrographia on free writing versus copying tasks in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 11 57–63.
Leverenz J.B., Quinn J.F., Zabetian C., Zhang J., Montine K.S., Montine T.J. (2009). Cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with Parkinson disease. Curr Top Med Chem.9(10):903-12.
Lieberman M. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109–137.
Lieberman P., Kako E., Friedman J., Tajchman G., FeldmanL. S. y Jiminez E. B.(1992). Speech production, syntax comprehension, and cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Language, 43, 169–189.
Miwa H. y Kondo T. (2005). Increased writing activity in Parkinson’s disease: a punding-like behavior? Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 11 (2005) 323–325.
Mortimer J. A., Pirozzolo F. J., Hansch E. C. y Webster D. (1982). Relationship of motor symptoms to intellectual deficitsin Parkinson disease. Neurology, 32, 133–137.
Murray L. L. (2000). Spoken language production in Huntington’s and Parkinson’s diseases. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 1350–1366.
Natsopoulos D., Grouios G., Bostantzopoulou G., Mentenopoulos G., Katsarou Z., y Logothetis J. (1993). Algorithmic andheuristic strategies in comprehension ofcomplement clauses bypatients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology, 31, 951–964.
Nutt J.y Wooten F. (2005) Clinical practice. Diagnosis and initial management of Parkinson’s disease. N Engl J Med. 353:1021-1027.
Ondo W., Satija, P. (2007) Withdrawal of visual feedback improves micrographia in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. Oct 31;22(14):2130-1.
Pahwa R., Lyons K. yKoller W. (2003). Handbook of Parkinson’s disease. Taylor y Francis e- Library. (Ed). New York, NY,EE.UU. Basel, Switzerland.
Pan American Health Organization (2008). Trastornos neurológicos: un serio desafío para la salud pública en las Américas y en todo el mundo. OMS
Pell M.y Leonard C. (2001). Susceptibility of nonverbal emotional processes to early Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders,16, S27.
Pérez S., Bradley J., Nouzeilles M., Merello M. (2003). Evaluación grafométrica de la escritura en pacientes con enfermedad de Párkinson idiopática y parkinsonismo secundario. Archivos de Neurología, Neurocirugía y Neuropsiquiatría, Vol. 10.(2).
Schrag A. y Schott J. (2006). Epidemiological, clinical, and genetic characteristics of early-onset parkinsonism. LancetNeurol 2006; 5: 355–63
Scorticati M.C. y Micheli F. (2006). Enfermedad de Párkinson y trastornos relacionados. Ed. Médica Panamericana. Buenos Aires, Argentina.
Skodda S, Rinsche H, Schlegel U. (2009). Progression of dysprosody in Parkinson’s disease over time a longitudinal study. Mov Disord. Apr 15;24(5):716-22.
Smalla J., Lyons K., y Kemper S. (1997). Grammatical abilities in Parkinson’s disease: evidence from written sentences Neuropsychologia. Dec;35(12):1571-6
Tedrus G.M., Fonseca L.C., Letro G.H., Bossoni A.S., Samara A.B. (2009). Dementia and mild cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. Jun; 67(2B): 423-7.
Teulings H, Contreras-Vidal J, Stelmach G, Adler C.(2002). Adaptation of handwriting size under distorted visual feedback in patients with Parkinson’s disease and elderly and young controls. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2002; 72: 315–324.
Torres O., León M., Álvarez E., Maragoto C., Álvarez L., Rivera O. (2001) Rehabilitación del lenguaje en la enfermedad de Parkinson. Revista Mexicana de Neurociencia. 2 (4): 241-244.
Tucha O., Mecklinger L., Thome J., Reiter A., Alders G.L., Sartor H., Naumann M., Lange K.W. (2005). Kinematic analysis of dopaminergic effects on skilled handwriting movements in Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm. May; 113 (5): 609-23.
Tyler L. K., yMarslen-Wilson, W. D. (1986). The effects ofcontext on the recognition of polymorphemic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 741–752.
Ullman M. T., Corkin S., Coppola M., Hickok G., Growdon J. H., Koroshetz W. J., et al. (1997). A neural dissociationwithin language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processedby the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9,266–276.
Van Gemmert A, Teulings H, Contreras-Vidal J, Stelmach G. (1999) Parkinsons disease and the control of size and speed in handwriting. Neuropsychologia 37. 1999. 684:94
Van Gemmert A., Adler C. y Stelmach G. (2003). Parkinson’s disease patients undershoot target size in handwriting and similar tasks. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry; 74:1502–1508.
Whiting E., Copland D., y Angw A.(2005). Verb and context processing in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurolinguistics,(18) 259–276.
Williams-Gray C., Foltynie T., Lewis S. y Barker R. (2006). Cognitive Deficits and Psychosis in Parkinson’s Disease. A Review of Pathophysiology and Therapeutic Options. CNS Drugs 20(6):477-505.
Zeuner K.E., Peller M., Knutzen A., Holler I., Münchau A., Hallett M. y cols. (2007). How to assess motor impairment in writer’s cramp. MovDisord. Jun 15; 22(8):1102-9.